Al-Jazeerah: Cross-Cultural Understanding

www.ccun.org

www.aljazeerah.info

News, May 3, 2014

 

Al-Jazeerah History

Archives 

Mission & Name  

Conflict Terminology  

Editorials

Gaza Holocaust  

Gulf War  

Isdood 

Islam  

News  

News Photos  

Opinion Editorials

US Foreign Policy (Dr. El-Najjar's Articles)  

www.aljazeerah.info

 

 

 

Editorial Note: The following news reports are summaries from original sources. They may also include corrections of Arabic names and political terminology. Comments are in parentheses.

Neanderthal Demise: An Archaeological Analysis of the Modern Human Superiority Complex

By Paola Villa and Wil Roebroeks

Plos One, April 30, 2014

Abstract

Neanderthals (Neandertals) are the best-studied of all extinct hominids (hominins), with a rich fossil record sampling hundreds of individuals, roughly dating from between 350,000 and 40,000 years ago. Their distinct fossil remains have been retrieved from Portugal in the west to the Altai area in central Asia in the east and from below the waters of the North Sea in the north to a series of caves in Israel in the south. Having thrived in Eurasia for more than 300,000 years, Neanderthals vanished from the record around 40,000 years ago, when modern humans entered Europe. Modern humans are usually seen as superior in a wide range of domains, including weaponry and subsistence strategies, which would have led to the demise of Neanderthals. This systematic review of the archaeological records of Neanderthals and their modern human contemporaries finds no support for such interpretations, as the Neanderthal archaeological record is not different enough to explain the demise in terms of inferiority in archaeologically visible domains. Instead, current genetic data suggest that complex processes of interbreeding and assimilation may have been responsible for the disappearance of the specific Neanderthal morphology from the fossil record.

Figures

Introduction

The demise of Neanderthals is one of the most debated issues in paleoanthropology. Their disappearance in the fossil record constitutes the biological part of a process of change that occurred in Europe and in the Near East between approximately 45 and 35 thousand years ago (ka) [1][2]. In western Eurasia, the process led to the replacement of an archaic population (Neanderthals) with Middle Paleolithic technologies by a population of modern humans (Homo sapiens) with Upper Paleolithic ones [3][5]. The study of this process of transition integrates data and scientists from a wide range of disciplines, including archaeologists, physical anthropologists, dating specialists, and increasingly so, geneticists.

Into the 1980’s many paleoanthropologists argued that the Neanderthals had evolved into modern humans (or modern Europeans) and that the Upper Paleolithic derived from the Middle Paleolithic Neanderthal culture. The opposite view assumed a single origin of modern humans and replacement of archaic populations, including Neanderthals, by modern humans immigrating from an unknown source area [6]. This view became widely accepted with advances in genetic studies and dating of fossils and sites in Africa, Europe and the Near East. In 1987 the work of Cann and colleagues [7] provided compelling mitochondrial evidence for a recent African origin of all modern humans. Later, the genetic evidence was supported by fossils which showed that Africans were far more modern looking than their Neanderthal contemporaries, with dates for the Omo Kibish 1 and Herto skulls in Ethiopia suggesting that the early modern human morphology emerged in East Africa possibly as early as 195,000 year ago [8][10]. There is now general agreement that modern humans originated in Africa, and subsequently expanded their range into the Near East and later into Europe. This is the core of the so-called Out-of-Africa hypothesis [11].

In tandem with these developments, archaeologists began looking for modern behavioral markers in African sites dated between 200,000 and 60,000 years ago. Many (see below) would now suggest that there is indeed evidence for significant behavioral and cognitive differences between Neanderthals and their African contemporaries, and that when early moderns encountered Neanderthals in Western Eurasia, these differences would have entailed the demise of the Neanderthals.

Hypotheses for the Demise of Neandertals

Virtually all explanations for the disappearance of the Neanderthals from the Eurasian record point in one way or another to the arrival of Homo sapiens, anatomically modern humans (AMH), in Europe and western Asia. Late Pleistocene dispersal events brought AMH into the ranges of other hominid populations outside of Africa. In recent years we have seen a series of publications with detailed maps purported to show the progress of modern humans and their new technology across various Eurasian landscapes populated by “archaic” hominids, including Neanderthals [12][15]. The source populations [16][19] and the routes supposedly taken by the advancing modern humans vary widely in these papers though, as do the chronologies for the supposed Out of Africa dispersal(s) of modern humans. Some archaeological estimates hypothesize an age of around 125 ka for the first AMH dispersals into the Arabian peninsula [20] and around 77 ka for India [21], while others suggest that AMH dispersal, thought to be associated with distinctively African technologies analogous to the “Howiesons Poort”, occurred only around 50–60 ka, i.e. after the Toba volcanic eruption at around 75 ka [22]. Genetic dates for the Out-of-Africa dispersal(s) of AMH also vary widely, between approximately 45 to 130 ka [23][25].

The disappearance of the archaic populations, including Neanderthals, is routinely explained in terms of the “superiority” of modern humans, who had developed in Africa the ability to evolve complex cultural traditions and had become equipped with cognitive capacities which allowed them to expand globally and replace all other hominids [26], [27]. Such interpretations have increasingly become based on proxies in the Middle Stone Age (MSA) archaeological record of sub-Saharan Africa which, compared to the Middle Paleolithic record of Europe and western Asia, would testify to superiority in a wide range of domains, either in Africa and/or upon arrival of Homo sapiens in the Neanderthal geographical ranges. These include inventiveness and capacity for innovation [11], [28], complex symbolic and linguistic abilities [29], [30], more efficient hunting strategies [31], exploitation of a broader range of resources including plants and aquatic ones [32], projectile technology [33][35], heat treatment of lithic raw materials [36], hafting technology [37], [38], planning capacities including larger scale social networks as shown by large transport distances of raw materials [39], environmental flexibility [40], memory capacity [41] as well as larger population sizes [42]. Inferiority in one or more of these domains has been at the core of many explanations for the demise of the Neanderthals.

Prior to the last decade the cultural attributes listed above were generally considered as exclusive manifestations of the western Eurasian Upper Paleolithic, as the result of a major behavioral revolution compared to the preceding Middle Paleolithic. Seen from a European or Near Eastern perspective, the Upper Paleolithic witnessed the introduction of new technologies, the ability to communicate symbolically, systematic use of body ornaments and various forms of mobile and rock art, by modern humans expanding from Africa into Eurasia, leading to the gradual replacement of non-modern populations, such as the Neanderthals [29], [43][45]. It was acknowledged that some of those features had emerged earlier in Africa, but the most complex technologies and art forms were seen as characteristic of the European Upper Paleolithic, thus clouding the issue of the source area where these innovations had taken place.

In 2000 McBrearty and Brooks [27] forcefully argued that the components of this “Upper Paleolithic revolution” were already visible in the African MSA, tens of thousands of year earlier. They suggested a gradual assembling of a package of modern human behavior in Africa, which was later exported to other regions of the Old World: a view contested by Klein [11], who stressed a later and punctuated emergence of “modern human behavior”. In 2003 D’Errico [46] reviewed the cultural attributes which McBrearty and Brooks saw as defining modernity. He argued that comparable traits also occur in the Neanderthal record and rejected the theory that behavioral “modernity” indicators are uniquely associated with Homo sapiens. Nevertheless, the behavioral markers described by McBrearty and Brooks have in recent years increasingly been used to explain the demise of the Neanderthals when modern humans expanded into their territories.

Non-archaeological data have also been called upon to explain the outcompeting of the large-bodied and big-brained Neanderthals by modern humans, but these fall in first instance out of the scope of this review (but see Discussion). The goal of this paper is to test the strength of the archaeology-derived hypotheses for Neanderthal extinction referred to above. While some of these hypotheses have been evaluated individually [47], ours is the first systematic study of a wide range explanations. It is timely too, given the large amount of new data generated by fieldwork in Africa, the resulting speculations on modern humans cognitive modernity [28], [30], and new insights into Neanderthal behavior and biology, including their biological affinity with modern humans. Genetic studies now suggest that the debate on the demise of the Neanderthals needs to be reframed in terms of some degree of interbreeding [23], [48], [49]. In that sense, Neanderthals did not go extinct, even though their distinctive morphology did disappear. We will return to this topic at the end of this paper.

Methods

Our evaluation of the key archaeology-derived explanations for the demise of the Neanderthals entails a comparative study of the archaeological record of Neanderthals and contemporary modern humans, i.e. AMH in Africa and Southwest Asia between 200 and 40 ka. To include younger periods would disregard the effects of cultural and technological evolution after the demise of the Neanderthals. The various competing models regarding the evolutionary disadvantages of Neanderthals are listed in Table 1 and are reviewed in detail in (Text S1 Hypotheses 1–11), where they are systematically described listing the specific hypothesis and supporting as well as refuting evidence.

Table 1. Hypotheses for the demise of Neanderthals (a).

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096424.t001

Transitional Industries

The so-called “transitional industries”, which show some similarities to late Middle Paleolithic (Mousterian) industries but also contain Upper Paleolithic forms and whose time range falls within a 45−35 ka interval, will not be discussed in detail here, for the following reasons:

  1. the makers of the Bohunician, Bachokirian, Szeletian and Streletskayan [in Central and Southeastern Europe and Russia) are not known yet (late Neanderthals or AMH?) and hence their status is ambiguous [50].
  2. Neanderthals are accepted by many–though not by all [51] - as the makers of the Châtelperronian, best known from the Grotte du Renne at Arcy-sur-Cure in France, excavated by Leroi-Gourhan and his team between 1949 and 1963 [52]. The interpretation of the industry, rich in distinctively “modern” cultural features such as ornaments and bone tools, has been the subject of heated debates. The controversies about whether the ornaments and bone tools were (i) an invention of Neanderthals [46], [53], (ii) the result of stratigraphic admixture of Neanderthal remains and Upper Paleolithic artifacts [54][57], or (iii) due to acculturation [28], [58], [59] have been going on since the acculturation hypothesis was most explicitly discussed in 1998 by D’Errico et al. [60]. The stratigraphic integrity of the Châtelperronian layers at the site has been reaffirmed in a recent paper [61], contra [62]. New radiocarbon dates of 44,970−44,520 cal BP for the start and 41,300−40,570 cal BP for the end of the Châtelperronian at Arcy and of 41,950−40,660 cal BP (all dates with probability at 68.2%) for the Saint Césaire Neanderthal suggest that the makers of the Châtelperronian ornaments were indeed Neanderthals [63]. However, the conflicting hypotheses of acculturation versus independent invention persist, as the dates appear to postdate or overlap in time with the arrival of early modern humans in Italy [5] and with dates for the Aurignacian in Germany [63; contra 2].

Early descriptions of Châtelperronian assemblages stressed a Mousterian component but the industry is now considered Upper Paleolithic in technology, although different from the Aurignacian, and the presence of Mousterian tools as due to syn-or post-depositional mixing [51]. The Châtelperronian lithic industry recently studied at the open-air site Canaule II in France [64] is also described as fully Upper Paleolithic, based on its technology and almost exclusive production of blades and backed points. In contrast to the Arcy site, the very large assemblage of Canaule II comes from a thin and unique layer, with its integrity and homogeneity confirmed by refitting. The absence of any Middle Paleolithic elements in this Châtelperronian assemblage again strongly suggests that the Châtelperronian, chronologically intermediate between the Middle and the Upper Paleolithic, is a unique entity, not the result of a mix of Middle and Upper Paleolithic artifacts.

  1. The Uluzzian, an Italian transitional industry also present in Greece and previously attributed to Neanderthals [60], [65], is now seen by some as a product of modern humans, on the basis of a study of two deciduous teeth from Grotta del Cavallo in southern Italy [5]. AMS dates on shell beads from Grotta del Cavallo yielded 45.010−43,380 cal BP for the lower Uluzzian layer. If the dates and the taxonomic attribution are accepted, they would extend the period of Neanderthal-modern human coexistence to some millennia. Neanderthals are thought to have persisted in southern Iberia until 37 ka, based on the dates for Middle Paleolithic assemblages there [66] and at other sites in Europe based on dates for Neanderthal remains at Spy (Belgium) and Vindija (Croatia). Elsewhere the dates for the Campanian Ignimbrite ash horizon, stratigraphically above several Proto-Aurignacian layers, situate the end of the Middle Paleolithic at about 40 ka (see The date of the demise).

The “transitional” industries are extremely relevant to understand the routes of migrations and expansion of AMH in Europe, the nature of cultural contacts between the local and immigrant populations and the onset of the Upper Paleolithic in those regions. However, we need more contextual (i.e. stratigraphic, technological and in some cases fossil) data before we can make accurate assessments of the evidence, e.g. in terms of the type of hominid authorship [62]. In the case of the Châtelperronian, attributed to Neanderthals by several scholars, we will review recent evidence from sites where stratigraphic admixture can be excluded and are less controversial than Grotte du Renne.

Results

Explanations for the demise of Neanderthals have been developed at various levels of abstraction, and include topics notoriously difficult to study in the archaeological record, such as “complex symbolic communication systems” [28], “fully syntactic language” [67] or “cognitive capacities” in general. Other hypotheses refer to behavioral domains which do leave clear traces in the archaeological record, provided the right taphonomic conditions prevail (Table 1; Text S1, Hypotheses 1–11).

Language and Symbolism

The archaeological record has been mined in various ways to produce evidence for symbolic aspects of human culture, with a strong focus on the emergence of language. Archaeological finds from the MSA have been used to build scenarios for the timing and location of the origin(s) of language. Several of these finds come from South Africa and include engraved pieces of ochre from Blombos Cave [68][69], Nassarius shells from the same location [70], and heated silcrete artefacts from the site of Pinnacle Point, said to testify to sophisticated pyrotechnological know-how by early modern humans [36]. Botha has shown the assumptions and series of inferential steps some of these authors had to make before being able to squeeze “language” out of their mute artifacts [71][72], see also [73][74] pinpointing the weak spots in the steps leading from observations about archaeological phenomena to statements about the presence of “fully syntactical language”. Moreover, recent data on Neandertal use of ochre and manganese as well as on Neanderthal production of pitch, the presence of transported and ochre-smeared shells, of ornaments such as eagle claws and perhaps bird feathers [75][78] (Text S1, Hypothesis 1), and the production of the specialized bone tools recently reported from two late Middle Paleolithic sites [79] (Text S3, Lissoirs) indicate no significant differences between the MSA data commonly used to create these more abstract explanations and the later Middle Paleolithic record.

The same applies to explanations regarding behavioral domains which do leave clear traces in the archaeological record. In our study none of the explanations listed in the introduction and in Table 1 proved to be supported by adequate archaeological data.

Hunting Methods and Diet

With the demise of the idea that Neanderthals were scavengers and ineffective hunters [80][82], the former interpretive framework has to some degree been reformulated in terms of Neanderthals inferiority in subsistence strategies and hunting weaponry for which, again, there is no support from the archaeological record (Text SI, Hypotheses 3–4). Neanderthals were by all means accomplished large game hunters, who survived in a wide range of environments subsisting by hunting a wide range of animals in a variety of topographical settings. In contrast to prevailing ideas [31], [83], their diet was not restricted to large and medium size herbivores only. Several sites document a broader diet, including aquatic foods, small fast game (birds, rabbits) as well as plant resources (SI Hypothesis 5). Likewise, the idea that spear throwers and bow and arrow were first developed in the MSA of South Africa before 60 ka and conferred substantive advantages on modern humans as they left Africa and encountered Neanderthals equipped with only hand-cast spears [33], [84] may be correct, but there is no solid archaeological evidence in its support (Text S1, Hypothesis 4.3).

Organized Use of Space

The same applies to purported differences in the use of space at the level of camp sites by AMH and Neanderthals, with organized use of space seen as typical for AMH. The South African MSA record has some cases of excellent preservation of plant materials in dry conditions, including possible bedding material recovered from 77 to 58 ka old deposits at Sibudu [85][86]. Some researchers have taken the presence of bedding material and “the deliberate use and organization of living space” to be “an important trait of culturally modern behavior” [87]. However, there exists good evidence for well-delimited activity areas at Neanderthal sites such as Kebara, Amud (Israel) and Tor Faraj (Jordan) as well as from several European sites where the task-specific areas are documented by refitting (Text S1, Hypothesis 3). Furthermore, bed building by great apes is a well-documented learned behavior, dependent on appropriate early experiences [88].

Capacity for Innovation

Another prominent scenario suggests that the archaeological record of sub-Saharan modern humans, to wit of the two main technocomplexes of the South African late MSA, the Still Bay (SB) and the Howiesons Poort (HP), indicates very dynamic and innovative phases which lasted less than 10,000 years each [26], [89]. These would constitute a striking contrast to the record of the Neanderthals, who supposedly lacked the capacities to innovate and “made the same kinds of tools for 200,000 years without ever tinkering with the basic components” [90]. Recently reported dates from the Diepkloof site (South Africa) are significantly complicating our views on cultural change in the Late Pleistocene there, however. According to these new dates [91][92] the SB and HP technocomplexes would have a much longer duration than previously envisaged [89], comparable to those of broadly contemporaneous Middle Paleolithic industries in Europe, which show clear spatio-temporal distributions (Table 2, Text S1 Hypothesis 2). Jacobs’ OSL age estimates for the SB and HP are considered controversial by some [93]. More dating work is clearly required, while systematic technological and typological analyses are necessary to dispel doubts about assemblage definition, especially for the MIS 5 occurrences.

Table 2. Dates of technological phases in the late Middle Paleolithic of Europe and in the late Middle Stone Age of South Africa (a).

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096424.t002

In contrast it is clear that the Post-HP technocomplex, characterized by unifacial points on flakes (Sibudu) or blades (Border Cave, Klasies), hard hammer percussion, rare presence of the Levallois technique and of formal tools on blades (Klasies) and flakes (esp. Rose Cottage and Sibudu), has a duration of about 20,000 years; even more if “transitional” or late MSA sites in South Africa, dated between 40 and 20 ka, are taken into account. These include three layers at Rose Cottage, dated between ca 30.8 and 27 ka, and Strathalan Cave B, with two layers dated between 29 and 25.7 ka [94]. OSL and ESR dates for post-HP assemblages are supported by AMS radiocarbon dates. Thus the pace of change and the evolutionary patterns of the European Upper Pleistocene record, which shows regional differentiation, cultural traditions and technological changes through time, are comparable to what is known from the African record. Technological and tool-type changes in the Mousterian industries precede by far the advent of Proto-Aurignacian and Aurignacian industries. Whatever dates are accepted for these industries [95], changes in Mousterian industries occurred long before 50 ka.

Size of Social Networks

Other workers have suggested that Neanderthals and AMH differed significantly in the sizes of their social networks. AMH larger-scale social networks are supposed to have acted as a buffer against environmental downturns, thus fostering long term survival. Such inferences are based on the translation of distances over which artifacts were transported in the deep past into statements about former mobility strategies, exchange systems and sizes of social networks. Yet it is almost impossible to differentiate between long distance transport as a signature of direct procurement as opposed to indirect acquisition, such as through trade or exchange networks [107]. Our review of the evidence (Text S1, Hypothesis 7) shows that as far as the archaeological record for raw material transfer distances is concerned, the MSA and the Middle Paleolithic record are not significantly different, despite of the obvious ecological differences between western Eurasia and Africa.

Hafting Procedures, Heat Treatment and Cognition

According to another hypothesis Neanderthals hafting of tools was a simple procedure, only using naturally available glues. Early modern human hafting techniques entailed complex procedures which required “abstract reasoning” and are hence indicative of modern cognition. According to Wynn and Coolidge [41] evidence for complex hafting procedures dates back to about 70 ka in South Africa. Replication experiments suggest that HP hunters used a mixture of plant gum, beeswax and powdered ochre to produce an adhesive that had to be carefully dried using fire [37]. However, from 200,000 years ago onward, European Neanderthals used fire to synthesize pitch from bark, through a process that involved distillation in the absence of oxygen and within a temperature interval of 340°C–400°C [108]. Pitch is not a naturally occurring glue; it is a man-made material produced using fire as a tool. Birch park pitches have been experimentally produced in small dug out and subsequently covered pits beneath camp fires [109], though in very small quantities only, leaving open the question how exactly Neanderthals produced their pitches. Two flakes associated with elephant remains at the Italian site of Campitello (Tuscany, Italy) were found enclosed in blackish organic material that was analyzed by gas chromatography/mass spectrometry and identified as a pitch obtained by a pyrolysis-type process of birch bark for hafting the flint flakes [110][111]. The Campitello finds date to the end of MIS 7. Comparable finds of birch bark pitch come from the German site Königsaue A, with an estimated age of 80 ka [112][113]. On basis of the stratigraphy of the site, the AMS dates of 43,800±2100 BP and 48,400±3700 BP cited in ref [113] should be considered minimum ages. Mania’s fieldwork at the site produced two pieces of pitch, one with fingerprints as well as the imprint of a stone tool and a wooden haft. Experimental studies show that production of pitch in the absence of air-tight pottery containers requires a high degree of technical knowledge.

According to Brown et al [36] heat treatment of silcrete at the South African site of Pinnacle Point at c. 72 ka and possibly as early as 164 ka indicates sophisticated knowledge of fire and elevated cognitive abilities that may have been a behavioral advantage on Neanderthals as early modern humans moved to Eurasia. The evidence of pitch production as early as 200 ka by European Neanderthals shows that those “elevated cognitive abilities” were not the exclusive domain of modern humans.

The straightforward scenario of superior AMH moving into Neanderthal territory is also complicated by the Late Pleistocene occupation history of the East Mediterranean Levant. AMH were present in that region between 80 and 130 ka, and created the Skuhl and Qafzeh record with its burials, pigments and personal ornaments [114], associated with a Middle Paleolithic lithic technology. Between 80 and 47 ka however, only Neanderthals are known from the fossil record of the Levant [115]. If the absence of fossil AMH in the record represents a true absence from the region, this could indicate that the Skuhl/Qafzeh hominids and their immediate descendants indeed may have “lacked the behavioral capacities that enabled subsequent modern humans to compete successfully against the Neanderthals” [115].

Discussion

We conclude that all the “archaeology-based” explanations for the demise of the Neanderthals reviewed here (Table 1, Text S1, Hypotheses 1–11) are flawed. They were based on much less data than we have available today and were at least in part the result of a long tradition of thinking in terms of Neanderthals-AMH dichotomies, steered by overstressing developments within the Upper Paleolithic of Europe, the record of which has become almost like a yardstick for modern human behavior (Text S2).

While the debate about AMH dispersal times and routes out of Africa is intense, based on a range of archaeological as well as genetic data, the archaeological record from the various continents does not provide strong support for any of the suggested routes nor any of the suggested factors in the demise of the Neanderthals. The very fact that the migration time estimates vary so widely suggests that we simply have no solid data; perhaps there was more than one migration event (in addition to the Last Interglacial limited expansion in the Levant), and in all probability the migrating groups did not have a strong cultural homogeneity. This may explain why we do not see clear archaeological signatures for AMH on the move.

Interestingly, the widely accepted date of 60 or 50 ka for the modern human expansion into Eurasia (following the earlier short-lived exodus in the Levant documented at Skuhl and Qafzeh) would rule out South Africa as the location for source populations for two reasons: (i) by 60 ka the HP tradition of backed tools made on blades and bladelets produced by soft stone hammer (supposedly associated with the AMH expansion) had given way to the post-HP assemblages characterized by a variety of flake tools and blades produced by hard hammer percussion but without backed blades [94], [103], [105], [116]; (ii) the Still Bay and HP populations were not larger than other MSA populations and might even have been smaller, thus excluding population pressure as the prime mover of the migration [117]. According to Klein [11] the Out of Africa expansion was underlain by a neural mutation that promoted the final development of the modern human brain. Direct evidence for this hypothesis may come from comparisons of Neandertal and modern human genomes.

In the recent past, much debate has been generated from the observation that Neanderthals began to produce a richer archaeological record, including bone tools, personal ornaments and use of manganese and ochre, at the time when AMH started colonizing Europe. Some interpreted this change in the record as the result of Neanderthal absorption of ideas and techniques from the incoming AMH. After having produced a rather monotonous record for almost 300,000 years, an independent invention of these new items just at the time of the arrival of AMH would have to be seen as an “impossible coincidence” [28]. However, as reviewed here, use of ochre, of personal ornaments, production of specialized bone tools and complex hafting techniques were part of the Neanderthal repertoire already before the arrival of AMH in western Eurasia.

The present review also suggests that some of the innovative technologies of the Protoaurignacian and of the Aurignacian may have developed out of a Middle Paleolithic base (for a similar viewpoint, see [118]). Some components that occur sporadically or episodically in Neanderthal and late MSA assemblages become much more common later, like pigment use, symbolic objects, extensive transport of raw materials and even specialized bone tools [79]. The same goes for another element, the intentional production of bladelets (<4 cm in length) from bladelet cores. Bladelets have been considered a discriminant factor between the Upper and Middle Paleolithic and therefore between AMH and Neanderthals [119]. Production of bladelets has been securely identified in French Mousterian assemblages, e.g. at Combe Grenal (layers 30–29 and layers 16 and 14), Champ Grand and Grotte Mandrin, and in Spain at sites such as El Castillo and Cueva Morin [120], [121]. All these assemblages belong to the final Mousterian, with the exception of Combe Grenal and Grotte Mandrin; at the latter site, a layer with blades, bladelets and microlithic points is overlain by five layers with flake-based Mousterian assemblages [121]. At Combe Grenal layers 29–30 have an estimated age of late MIS 4, i.e. around 60 ka. Bladelets and bladelet cores are not abundant (5% of the assemblage at Combe Grenal layers 29–30), yet they show that Neanderthals, like late MSA humans and the makers of the Protoaurignacian, mastered the technology of bladelet production, albeit using methods different from the HP small blade technology. It is their frequency, not cognition or technical competence, that distinguishes AMH bladelet production from that of Neanderthals [120]. The techniques and methods of bladelet making in the Mousterian are different from those of the Protoaurignacian, just as the kind of possible symbolic objects are also different (use of raptor claws; on perforated or grooved animal teeth (see Text SI, Hypothesis 1). Perhaps the nature of the contacts should be seen in terms of diffusion of ideas rather than as face to face interaction and the copying of specific objects [122]. The occurrence of Dufour bladelets (often used as projectile elements in the Aurignacian and the Protoaurignacian) with very specific techniques of manufacture in the Châtelperronian of Quincay is interpreted in a similar way, as a form of low-degree social interaction between Neanderthals and modern humans [123].

The Date of the Demise

Various new dates support the idea of some chronological overlap between AMH and Neandertals, which may have enabled interbreeding and cultural interaction in western Europe: AMS dates on ultrafiltered bone collagen from the Châtelperronian layers X and IX of Grotte du Renne at Arcy, c. 44 to 41 kyr cal BP; the date of the Saint-Césaire Neandertal at 41.9−40.6 kyr cal BP [63]; the fact that the Protoaurignacian at the Italian sites of Castelcivita and Serino is overlain by the Campanian Ignimbrite tephra, dated to 39.28±0.11 ka by 40Ar/39Ar [124]; the modeled age ranges of c. 41.5−39.9 kyr cal BP of several radiocarbon-dated Proto-Aurignacian sites [95]; the date of the Oase 2 early modern cranium at c. 40 ka [125]; the AMS dates for the Neandertal child from Spy cave (Belgium), 36,870 to 38,494 and 37,297 to 40,490 cal BP [126]; the AMS dates for the Vindija (Croatia) Neandertal remains at c. 38 kyr cal BP [127], [128]; the 37.4 ka cal BP date for the final Mousterian level of Cueva Antón in southeastern Spain [75]. Even if we do not consider dates judged by some as controversial such as (i) the AMS dates on shell beads for the layer containing the modern human teeth at Grotta del Cavallo at 45.010−43,380 cal BP [5, contra 2]; (ii) the dates for the Kent’s Cavern modern human maxilla [3, contra 2, 129]; and (iii) the dates for the Aurignacian at Geissenklösterle at c. 42 kyr cal BP [4, contra 2], some millennia of overlap are indicated The latest Neanderthal currently known from the Levant is the adult male skeleton from Amud Cave (Israel) with an ESR date of 53±8 ka on tooth enamel [130].

Interbreeding and Assimilation

For some authors replacement and supposedly rapid extinction of Neanderthals can be explained only in terms of substantial cognitive, technological and demographic differences between the Neanderthals and AMH [42], [131]. But, as we tried to show here, the Neanderthal archaeological record was not different enough to explain their demise in terms of inferiority in archaeologically visible domains. Thus, if Neanderthals were not technologically and cognitively “disadvantaged”, how can we explain that they did not survive?

Some modern human-like anatomical characteristics are said to occur in late Neanderthal fossils (as in the Vindja, St. Césaire and Riparo Mezzena late Neanderthals [132], [133] and refs therein) and vice versa some Neanderthal features are present in early specimens of modern humans in Europe [134], [135] supporting a hypothesis of some degree of admixture between the two groups. However, until recently the morphological evidence of admixture was often dismissed. In 2010 a draft sequence of the Neanderthal nuclear DNA provided clear evidence of interbreeding between Neanderthals and modern humans [48], estimating that Neanderthal inheritance makes up 1–4% of the genomes of people outside of Africa. A revised estimate based on a high-coverage sequence of a Neanderthal from the Altai Mountains now suggests 1.5–2.1% [49]. Genes of Neanderthals may have been favored through natural selection, and possibly played a role in the development of the immune system of modern humans [136] or in UV-light adaptations [137]. According to [138] gene flow from Neanderthals to modern humans occurred between 47,000 and 65,000 years ago, and most likely happened at the time when Neanderthals and modern humans encountered each other in Europe and the Middle East around 50,000 years ago.

In sum, interbreeding and assimilation, the tenants of a model first proposed by Fred Smith [139] are now supported by genetic data [134], [140]. It can be argued that the level of interbreeding may have been too limited to support an assimilation scenario. An interestingly parallel to this complex situation can be found in another “revolution”, the so-called Neolithic Revolution [43], [141], which does not feature explanations in terms of “cognitive” differences. The first farmers swept into Europe from the Near East at about 7500y BP displacing the local Late Mesolithic hunter-gatherers. But the Mesolithic hunter-gatherers, who cannot be described as cognitively inferior, were not submerged by hordes of farmers. Farmers and foragers coexisted for thousands of years in NW Europe; in Central Europe local hunter-gatherers adopted farming but in southern Scandinavia local foragers retained the Mesolithic lifestyle for c. 1500 years after farming arrived in Central Europe [142], [143]. Cultural contact is suggested by clear continuities in flint technology between the Mesolithic and early Neolithic in the region. After a very complex pattern of expansions and genetic shifts of the last 8,000 years the hunter-gatherer mitochondrial DNA haplogroups form 16% of the present-day Central European genetic composition [143]. It would take at least one millennium between the first arrival of immigrants and a notable increase in their population size.

The original Neanderthal contribution to modern human biology may have been larger 40,000 years ago - equivalent to 2000 generations (with generation time at 20 years) – than estimates based on genomic regions of present-day humans suggest [144], [145]. Interbreeding of Neanderthals and modern humans may have helped modern humans to adapt to non-African environments but also introduced alleles that were not tolerated and contributed to male hybrid sterility thus reducing the proportion of Neanderthal ancestry of the period of contact to that seen today [144].

Mitochondrial genetic diversity of eight early modern European humans dated to ca 38,000 to 4,500 (14C cal BP, from Kostienki 14 to Őtzi The Iceman) is 1.5 times higher than that of five European Neanderthals spanning the time to 38 to 70 ka [23], [146]. The high coverage genome of the Altai Neanderthal [49] also suggests low genetic diversity which could indicate small population sizes (see Text S1 Hypothesis 8 for archaeological data). These genetic data suggest that differences in population sizes between the “resident” Neanderthals and incoming AMH populations may have been a contributing factor in the absorption of Neanderthal populations [23]. The momentous cultural changes that followed the arrival of AMH in Western Eurasia were not uniquely due to the residents’ cognitive or technological inferiority causing rapid and total replacement. The Neanderthal demise appears to have resulted from a complex and protracted process [147] including multiple dynamic factors such as low population density, interbreeding with some cultural contact, possible male hybrid sterility and contraction in geographic distribution [148] followed by genetic swamping and assimilation by the increasing numbers of modern immigrants.

Conclusion

In a review of the MSA and Middle Paleolithic archaeological record we have shown that inferred markers of modern human cognitive and behavioral capacities have a greater time depth in the Middle Palaeolithic record than commonly acknowledged. We have found no data in support of the supposed technological, social and cognitive inferiority of Neanderthals (Neandertals) compared to their AMH contemporaries. The results of our study imply that single-factor explanations for the disappearance of the Neanderthals are not warranted any more, and that their demise was clearly more complex than many archaeology-based scenarios of “cognitive inferiority” reviewed here seem to suggest. This has implications beyond the field of archaeology per se: archaeologists’ characterizations of Neanderthals as cognitively inferior to modern humans [149] have created an interpretive framework within which subtle biological differences between Neanderthals and modern humans tend to be over-interpreted (see for instance [150].

After 40,000 years and 2000 generations, the Neanderthal fraction in non-African modern human genomes still constitutes a substantial legacy from these ancient hominids (hominins) who differed from contemporary AMHs in both genotypes and phenotypes [151] but whose archeological record was not different enough to support the purported cognitive “gap” between them and their contemporary modern humans.

Supporting Information

Text_S1.doc

Hypotheses 1–11. Data on the various competing models on the evolutionary disadvantages of Neandertals, presented as Hypotheses 1 to 11, systematically described listing each specific hypothesis and supporting as well as refuting evidence.

Text S1.

Hypotheses 1–11. Data on the various competing models on the evolutionary disadvantages of Neandertals, presented as Hypotheses 1 to 11, systematically described listing each specific hypothesis and supporting as well as refuting evidence.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096424.s001

(DOC)

Text S2.

A single package of modern behavior?

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096424.s002

(DOC)

Text S3.

Lissoirs.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096424.s003

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

We thank three reviewers for their constructive comments. P.V. is grateful to Payson Sheets, John Hoffecker, Douglas Bamforth and Gerardo Gutierrez for useful suggestions. Marie Soressi and Alexander Verpoorte gave valuable feedback on previous versions of the paper.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: PV WR. Analyzed the data: PV WR. Wrote the paper: PV WR. Performed research: PV WR.

References

  1. 1. Douka K, Bergman CA, Hedges REM, Wesselingh FP, Higham TFG (2013) Chronology of Ksar Akil (Lebanon) and implications for the colonization of Europe by anatomically modern humans. PLoS One 8(9) e72931.
  2. 2. Zilhão J (2013) Neandertal-Modern Human contact in Western Eurasia: issues of dates, taxonomy and cultural associations. In: Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K editors. Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and Modern Humans. Volume 1: Cultural perspectives, eds (Springer, Tokyo), pp21–57.
  3. 3. Higham T, Compton T, Stringer C, Jacobi R, Shapiro B, et al. (2011) The earliest evidence for anatomically modern humans in northwestern Europe. Nature 479: 521–524. doi: 10.1038/nature10484
  4. 4. Higham T, Basell L, Wood R, Bronk Ramsey C, Conard NJ (2012) Testing models for the beginnings of the Aurignacian and the advent of figurative art and music: The radiocarbon chronology of Geiβenklösterle. J Hum Evol 62: 664–676. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.03.003
  5. 5. Benazzi S, Douka K, Fornai C, Bauer CC, Kullmer O (2011) Early dispersal of modern humans in Europe and implications for Neanderthal behaviour. Nature 479: 525–528. doi: 10.1038/nature10617
  6. 6. Howells WW (1976) Explaining modern man: Evolutionists versus migrationists. J Hum Evol 5: 477–495. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(76)90088-9
  7. 7. Cann RL, Stoneking M, Wilson AC (1987) Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution. Nature 325: 31–36. doi: 10.1038/325031a0
  8. 8. White TD, Asfaw B, DeGusta D, Gilbert H, Richards GD, et al. (2003) Pleistocene Homo sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature 423: 742–752. doi: 10.1038/nature01669
  9. 9. Klein RG (2009) The Human Career: Human Biological and Cultural Evolution University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
  10. 10. Aubert M, Pike AWG, Stringer C, Bartsiokas A, Kinsley L, et al. (2012) Confirmation of a late middle Pleistocene age for the Omo Kibish 1 cranium by direct uranium-series dating. J Hum Evol 63: 704–710. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.07.006
  11. 11. Klein RG (2008) Out of Africa and the Evolution of Human Behavior. Evol Anthropol 17: 267–281. doi: 10.1002/evan.20181
  12. 12. Mellars P (2006) Going east: New genetic and archaeological perspectives on the modern human colonization of Eurasia. Science 313: 796–800. doi: 10.1126/science.1128402
  13. 13. Mellars P (2006) A new radiocarbon revolution and the dispersal of modern humans. Nature 439: 931–935. doi: 10.1038/nature04521
  14. 14. Bar-Yosef O, Belfer-Cohen A (2013) Following Pleistocene road signs of human dispersals across Eurasia. Quat Int 285: 30–43. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2011.07.043
  15. 15. Cann RL (2013) Y Weigh In Again on Modern Humans. Science 341(6145): 465–467. doi: 10.1126/science.1242899
  16. 16. Campbell MC, Tishkoff SA (2010) The Evolution of Human Genetic and Phenotypic Variation in Africa. CB 20(4): R166–R173. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.050
  17. 17. Henn BM, Gignoux CR, Jobin M, Granka JM, Macpherson JM, et al. (2011) Hunter-gatherer genomic diversity suggests a southern African origin for modern humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108: 5154–5162. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1017511108
  18. 18. Hublin JJ, Klein RG (2011) Northern Africa could also have housed the source population for living humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 08(28): E277. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105710108
  19. 19. Schlebusch CM, Skoglund P, Sjödin P, Gattepaille LM, Hernandez D, et al. (2012) Genomic Variation in Seven Khoe-San Groups Reveals Adaptation and Complex African History. Science 338: 374–379. doi: 10.1126/science.1227721
  20. 20. Armitage SJ, Jasim SA, Marks AE, Parker AG, Usik VI, et al. (2011) The Southern Route ‘Out of Africa’: Evidence for an Early Expansion of Modern Humans into Arabia. Science 331: 453–456. doi: 10.1126/science.1199113
  21. 21. Petraglia MD, Ditchfield P, Jones S, Korisettar R, Pal JN (2012) The Toba volcanic super-eruption, environmental change, and hominin occupation history in India over the last 140,000 years. Quat Int 258: 119–134. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2011.07.042
  22. 22. Mellars P, Gori KC, Carr M, Soares PA, Richards MB (2013) Genetic and archaeological perspectives on the initial modern human colonization of southern Asia. Proc Natl Acad Sci 110: 10699–10704. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1306043110
  23. 23. Fu Q, Mittnik A, Johnson PLF, Bos K, Lari M, et al. (2013) A Revised Timescale for Human Evolution Based on Ancient Mitochondrial Genomes. CB 23: 553–559. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.044
  24. 24. Fernandes V, Alshamali F, Alves M, Costa MD, Pereira JB, et al. (2012) The Arabian Cradle: Mitochondrial Relicts of the First Steps along the Southern Route out of Africa. Am J Hum Genet 90(2): 347–355. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.12.010
  25. 25. Scally A, Durbin R (2012) Revising the human mutation rate: implications for understanding human evolution. Nat Rev Genet 13: 745–753. doi: 10.1038/nrg3295
  26. 26. Mellars P (2006) Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 years ago? A new model. Proc Natl Acad Sci 103: 9381–9386. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510792103
  27. 27. McBrearty S, Brooks AS (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J Hum Evol 39: 453–563. doi: 10.1006/jhev.2000.0435
  28. 28. Mellars PA (2005) The Impossible Coincidence. A Single-Species Model for the Origins of Modern Human Behavior in Europe. Evol Anthropol 14: 12–27. doi: 10.1002/evan.20037
  29. 29. Mellars P (1989) Major Issues in the Emergence of Modern Humans. Curr Anthropol 30: 349–385. doi: 10.1086/203755
  30. 30. Conard NJ (2010) Cultural modernity: Consensus or conundrum? Proc Natl Acad Sci 107: 7621–7622. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001458107
  31. 31. Marean CW (2005) From the tropics to the colder climates: contrasting faunal exploitation adaptations of modern humans and Neanderthals. In: d’Errico F, Backwell L, editors. From Tools to Symbols. From Early Hominids to Modern Humans, (Witwatersrand University Press, Johannesburg), pp333–371.
  32. 32. Marean CW, Bar-Matthews M, Bernatchez J, Fisher E, Goldberg P, et al. (2007) Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene. Nature 449: 905–908. doi: 10.1038/nature06204
  33. 33. Brown KS, Marean CM, Jacobs Z, Schoville BJ, Oestmo S, et al. (2012) An early and enduring advanced technology originating 71,000 years ago in South Africa. Nature 491: 590–593. doi: 10.1038/nature11660
  34. 34. Shea JJ, Sisk ML (2010) Complex projectile technology and Homo sapiens dispersal into western Eurasia. PaleoAnthropology 2010: 100–122.
  35. 35. Shea JJ (2009) The ecological impact of projectile weaponry in Late Pleistocene human evolution. In: Hublin JJ, Richards MP, editors. The evolution of hominid diets: integrating approaches to the study of Paleolithic subsistence, Springer, Dordrecht, pp189–199.
  36. 36. Brown KS, Marean CW, Herries AIR, Jacobs Z, Tribolo C, et al. (2009) Fire As an Engineering Tool of Early Modern Humans. Science 325: 859–862. doi: 10.1126/science.1175028
  37. 37. Wadley L, Hodgskiss T, Grant M (2009) Implications for complex cognition from the hafting of tools with compound adhesives in the Middle Stone Age, South Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106: 9590–9594. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900957106
  38. 38. Wynn T (2009) Hafted spears and the archaeology of mind. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106: 9544–9545. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904369106
  39. 39. Nash D, Coulson S, Staurset S, Ullyott JS, Babutsi M, et al. (2013) Provenancing of silcrete raw materials indicates long-distance transport to Tsodilo Hills, Botswana, during the Middle Stone Age. J Hum Evol 64: 280–288. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.01.010
  40. 40. Hockett B, Haws JA (2005) Nutritional ecology and the human demography of Neandertal extinction. Quat Int 137: 21–34. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2004.11.017
  41. 41. Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2011) The implications of the working memory model for the evolution of modern cognition. Int J Evol Biol 2011: 741357. doi: 10.4061/2011/741357
  42. 42. Mellars P, French JC (2011) Tenfold Population Increase in Western Europe at the Neandertal-to-Modern Human Transition. Science 333: 623–627. doi: 10.1126/science.1206930
  43. 43. Bar-Yosef O (1998) On the Nature of Transitions: the Middle to Upper Palaeolithic and the Neolithic Revolution. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 8: 141–163. doi: 10.1017/s0959774300001815
  44. 44. Bar-Yosef O (2002) The Upper Palaeolithic Revolution. Annu Rev Anthropol 31: 363–393. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.31.040402.085416
  45. 45. Mellars P (1998) Neanderthals, Modern Humans and the Archaeological Evidence for Language. In: Jablonski NG, Aiello LC, editors. The origin and diversification of language, Memoirs of the California Academy of Sciences, California Academy of Sciences, vol 24, pp89–115.
  46. 46. D’Errico F (2003) The invisible frontier. A multiple species model for the origin of behavioral modernity. Evol Anthropol 12: 188–202. doi: 10.1002/evan.10113
  47. 47. Dogandžić T, McPherron SP (2013) Demography and the demise of Neandertals: A comment on ‘Tenfold population increase in Western Europe at the Neandertal-to-modern human transition’. J Hum Evol 64: 311–313. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.01.005
  48. 48. Green RE, Krause J, Briggs AW, Maricic T, Stenzek U, et al. (2010) A Draft Sequence of the Neandertal Genome. Science 328: 710–722.
  49. 49. Prüfer K, Racimo F, Patterson N, Jay F, Sankararaman S, et al. (2014) The complete genome sequence of a Neanderthal from the Altai Mountains. Nature 505: 43–49. doi: 10.1038/nature12886
  50. 50. Hublin JJ (2012) The earliest modern human colonization of Europe. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109: 13471–13472. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211082109
  51. 51. Bar-Yosef O, Bordes JG (2010) Who were the makers of the Chatelperronian culture? J Hum Evol 59: 586–593. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.06.009
  52. 52. Schmider B (2002) L’Aurignacien de la grotte du Renne: les fouilles d’André Leroi-Gourhan à Arcy-sur-Cure, Yonne. Editions CNRS, Paris, 309 p.
  53. 53. Zilhão J, d’Errico F, Border JG, Lenoble A, Texier JP, et al. (2006) Analyis of Aurignacian Interstratification at the Châtelperronian Type-site and Implications for the Behavioral Modernity of Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci 103: 12643–12648. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605128103
  54. 54. Higham T, Jacobi R, Julien M, David F, Basell L, et al. (2010) Chronology of the Grotte du Renne (France) and implications for the context of ornaments and human remains within the Châtelperronian. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107: 20234–20239. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007963107
  55. 55. Mellars P (2010) Neanderthal symbolism and ornament manufacture: The bursting of a bubble? Proc Natl Acad Sci 107(47): 20147–20148. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014588107
  56. 56. Taborin Y (1998) Comment to d’Errico F, Zilhão J, Julien M, Baffier D, Pelegrin J, Neandertal acculturation in Western Europe? A critical review of the evidence and its interpretation. Curr Anthropol 39: 28–29. doi: 10.1086/204689
  57. 57. White R (2002) Observations technologiques sur les objets de parure. In: Schmider B editor. L’Aurignacien de la grotte du Renne. Les fouilles d’André Leroi-Gourhan à Arcy-sur-Cure (Yonne). CNRS, Paris, pp257–266.
  58. 58. Mellars P (1999) Châtelperronian chronology and the case for Neandertal/modern human “acculturation” in western Europe. Curr Anthropol 40(3): 341–350.
  59. 59. Hublin J-J, Spoor F, Braun M, Zonneveld F, Condemi S (1996) A Late Neanderthal associated with Upper Palaeolithic artefacts. Nature 381: 224–226. doi: 10.1038/381224a0
  60. 60. d’Errico F, Zilhão J, Baffier D, Julien M, Pelegrin J (1998) Neandertal acculturation in Western Europe? A critical review of the evidence and its interpretation. Curr Anthropol 39: S1–S44. doi: 10.1086/204689
  61. 61. Caron F, d’Errico F, Del Moral P, Santos F, Zilhão J (2011) The Reality of Neandertal Symbolic Behavior at the Grotte du Renne, Arcy-sur-Cure, France. PloS One 6(6) e21545.
  62. 62. Higham T, Brock F, Bronk Ramsey C, Davies W, Wood R, et al. (2011) Chronology of the site of Grotte du Renne, Arcy-sur-Cure, France: implications for Neanderthal symbolic behaviour. Before Farming 2011(2): 1–9.
  63. 63. Hublin J-J, Talamo S, David F, Connet N, Bodu P, et al. (2012) Radiocarbon dates from the Grotte du Renne and Saint-Césaire support a Neandertal origin for the Châtelperronian. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109: 18743–18748. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212924109
  64. 64. Bachellerie F, Bordes JG, Morala A, Pelegrin J (2007) Etude typo-technologique et spatiale de remontages lithiques de Canaule II, site Châtelperronien de plein-air en Bergeracois (Creysse, Dordogne). Paléo 19: 259–280.
  65. 65. d’Errico F, Borgia V, Ronchitelli A (2012) Uluzzian bone technology and its implications for the origin of behavioural modernity. Quat Int 259: 59–71. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2011.03.039
  66. 66. Zilhão J, Davis SJM, Duarte C, Soares AMM, Steier P, et al.. (2010) Pego do Diabo (Loures, Portugal): Dating the Emergence of Anatomical Modernity in Westernmost Eurasia. PLoS ONE 5(1) e8880.
  67. 67. Henshilwood CS, d’Errico F, Vanhaeren M, van Niekerk K, Jacobs Z (2004) Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa. Science 304: 404. doi: 10.1126/science.1095905
  68. 68. Henshilwood CS, d’Errico F, Yates R, Jacobs Z, Tribolo C, et al. (2002) Emergence of Modern Human Behavior: Middle Stone Age Engravings from South Africa. Science 295: 1278–1280. doi: 10.1126/science.1067575
  69. 69. Henshilwood CS, d’Errico F, Watts I (2009) Engraved ochres from the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. J Hum Evol 57: 27–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.01.005
  70. 70. d’Errico F, Henshilwood C, Vanhaeren M, van Niekerk K (2005) Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. J Hum Evol 48: 3–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.09.002
  71. 71. Botha R (2008) Prehistoric shell beads as a window on language evolution. Language and Communication 28: 197–212. doi: 10.1016/j.langcom.2007.05.002
  72. 72. Botha R (2010) On the Soundness of Inferring Modern Language from Symbolic Behaviour. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 20: 345–356. doi: 10.1017/s0959774310000454
  73. 73. Balari S, Benítez-Burraco A, Camps M, Longa VM, Lorenzo G, et al. (2011) The archaeological record speaks: bridging anthropology and linguistics. Int J Evol Biol 2011(Article ID 382679): 1–17. doi: 10.4061/2011/382679
  74. 74. Botha R (2009) Theoretical underpinnings of inferences about language evolution: the syntax used at Blombos Cave. In: Botha R, Knight C, editors. The Cradle of Language. Oxford University Press, New York, pp93–111.
  75. 75. Zilhão J, Angelucci DE, Badal-García E, d’Errico F, Daniel F, et al. (2010) Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107: 1023–1028. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914088107
  76. 76. Peresani M, Vanhaeren M, Quaggiotto E, Queffelec A, d’Errico F (2013) An ochred fossil marine shell from the Mousterian of Fumane Cave. PLoS One 8(7) e68572.
  77. 77. Morin E, Laroulandie V (2012) Presumed Symbolic Use of Diurnal Raptors by Neanderthals. PLoS ONE 7(3) e32856.
  78. 78. Roebroeks W, Sier M, Kellberg Nielsen T, De Loecker D, Pares JM, et al. (2012) Use of red ochre by early Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109: 1889–1894. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112261109
  79. 79. Soressi M, McPherron S, Lenoir M, Dogandžic T, Goldberg P, et al. (2013) Neandertals made the first specialized bone tools in Europe. Proc Natl Acad Sci 110: 14186–14190. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302730110
  80. 80. Marean CW, Assefa Z (1999) Zooarchaeological Evidence for the Faunal Exploitation Behavior of Neanderthals and Early Modern Humans. Evol Anthropol 8: 22–37. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1520-6505(1999)8:1<22::aid-evan7>3.0.co;2-f
  81. 81. Villa P, Soto E, Santonja M, Pérez-González A, More R, et al. (2005) New data from Ambrona: closing the hunting versus scavenging debate Quat Int. 126–128: 223–250. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2004.03.001
  82. 82. Speth JD (2012) Middle Palaeolithic subsistence in the Near East: zooarchaeological perspectives – past, present and future. Before Farming 2012/2: 1–45.
  83. 83. O’Connell JF (2006) How did Modern Humans displace Neanderthals? Insights from hunter-gatherer Ethnography and Archaeology. In: Conard NJ, editor. When Neanderthals and Modern Humans Met. Kerns Verlag, Tübingen, pp43–64.
  84. 84. McBrearty S (2012) Sharpening the mind. Science 491: 531–532. doi: 10.1038/nature11751
  85. 85. Goldberg P, Miller CE, Schiegl S, Ligouis B, Berna F, et al. (2009) Bedding, hearths, and site maintenance in the Middle Stone Age of Sibudu Cave, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Archaeol Anthropol Sci 1: 95–122. doi: 10.1007/s12520-009-0008-1
  86. 86. Wadley L, Sievers C, Bamford M, Goldberg P, Berna F, et al. (2011) Middle Stone Age Bedding Construction and Settlement Patterns at Sibudu, South Africa. Science 334: 1388–1391. doi: 10.1126/science.1213317
  87. 87. Lombard M (2012) Thinking through the Middle Stone Age of sub-Saharan Africa. Quat Int 270: 140–155. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2012.02.033
  88. 88. Videan EN (2006) Bed-building in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the importance of early rearing. Am J Primatol 68: 745–751. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20265
  89. 89. Jacobs Z, Roberts RG, Galbraith RF, Deacon HJ, Grün R, et al. (2008) Ages for the Middle Stone Age of Southern Africa: implications for human behavior and dispersal. Science 322: 733–735. doi: 10.1126/science.1162219
  90. 90. Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2008) A Stone-Age Meeting of Minds. Am Sci 96: 44–51. doi: 10.1511/2008.69.44
  91. 91. Tribolo C, Mercier N, Valladas H, Joron JL, Guibert P, et al. (2013) OSL and TL dating of the Middle Stone Age sequence at Diepkloof Rock Shelter (South Africa): a clarification. J Archaeol Sci 40: 3401–3411. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2012.12.001
  92. 92. Porraz G, Texier PJ, Archer W, Piboule M, Rigaud JP, et al. (2013) Technological successions in the Middle Stone Age sequence of Diepkloof Rock Shelter, Western Cape, South Africa. J Archaeol Sci 40: 3376–3400. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2013.02.012
  93. 93. Guérin G, Murray AS, Jain M, Thomsen KJ, Mercier N (2013) How confident are we in the chronology of the transition between Howieson’s Poort and Still Bay? J Hum Evol 64: 314–317. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.01.006
  94. 94. Villa P, Soriano S, Tsanova T, Degano I, Higham TFG, et al. (2012) Border Cave and the beginning of the Later Stone Age in South Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109: 13208–13213. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202629109
  95. 95. Banks WE, d’Errico F, Zilhão J (2013) Human–climate interaction during the Early Upper Paleolithic: testing the hypothesis of an adaptive shift between the Proto-Aurignacian and the Early Aurignacian. J Hum Evol 64: 39–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.10.001
  96. 96. Porraz G, Parkington JE, Rigaud JP, Miller CR, Poggenpoel C, et al. (2013) The MSA sequence of Diepkloof and the history of southern African Late Pleistocene populations. J Archaeol Sci 40: 3542–3552. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2013.02.024
  97. 97. Guibert P, Bechtel F, Bourguignon L, Brenet M, Couchoud I, et al.. (2008) Une base de données pour la chronologie du Paléolithique moyen dans le Sud-Ouest de la France. In: Jaubert J, Bordes J-G, Ortega I editors. Les sociétés du Paléolithique dans un Grand Sud-ouest de la France: nouveaux gisements, nouveaux résultats, nouvelles méthodes. Mémoire de la Société Préhistorique Française, CNRS, Vol XLVII, pp19–40.
  98. 98. Jöris O (2004) Zur chronostratigraphischen Stellung der spätmittelpaläolithischen Keilmessergruppen. Bericht der Römisch-Germanischen Kommission 84: 49–153.
  99. 99. Jöris O (2006) Bifacially backed knives (Keilmesser) in the Central European Middle Palaeolithic. In: Goren-Inbar N, Sharon G, editors. Axe Age: Acheulian Tool-making from Quarry to Discard. Equinox Publishing Ltd., London, pp287–310.
  100. 100. Richter D, Hublin JJ, Jaubert J, McPherron S, Soressi M, et al. (2012) Thermoluminescence dates for the Middle Palaeolithic site of Chez-Pinaud Jonzac (France). J Archaeol Sci 40: 1176–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2012.09.003
  101. 101. Jacobs Z, Wintle AG, Duller GAT, Roberts RG, Wadley L (2008) New ages for the post-Howiesons Poort, late and final Middle Stone Age at Sibudu, South Africa. J Archaeol Sci 35: 1790–1807. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2007.11.028
  102. 102. Grün R, Beaumont P, Tobias PV, Eggins S (2003) On the age of Border Cave 5 human mandible. J Hum Evol 45: 155–168. doi: 10.1016/s0047-2484(03)00102-7
  103. 103. Villa P, Soriano S, Teyssandier N, Wurz S (2010) The Howiesons Poort and MSA III at Klasies River main site, Cave 1A. J Archaeol Sci 37: 630–655. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2009.10.028
  104. 104. Faith JT (2013) Taphonomic and palaeoecological change in the large mammal sequence from Boomplaas Cave, western Cape, South Africa. J Hum Evol 65: 715–730. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.09.001
  105. 105. Soriano S, Villa P, Wadley L (2007) Blade technology and tool forms in the Middle Stone Age of South Africa: the Howiesons Poort and post-Howiesons Poort at Rose Cottage Cave. J Archaeol Sci 34: 681–703. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2006.06.017
  106. 106. Pienaar M, Woodborne S, Wadley L (2008) Optically stimulated luminescence dating at Rose Cottage Cave. S Afr J Sci 104: 65–70.
  107. 107. Brantingham PJ (2006) Measuring Forager Mobility. Curr Anthropol 47: 435–459. doi: 10.1086/503062
  108. 108. Roebroeks W, Villa P (2011) On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108: 5209–5214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018116108
  109. 109. Palmer F (2007) Die Entstehung von Birkenpech in einer Feuerstelle unter paläolithischen Bedingungen. Mitteilungen der Gesellschaft für Urgeschichte 16: 75–83.
  110. 110. Mazza PPA, Martini F, Sala B, Magi M, Colombini MP, et al. (2006) A new Palaeolithic discovery: tar-hafted stone tools in a European Mid-Pleistocene bone-bearing bed. J Archaeol Sci 33: 1310–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2006.01.006
  111. 111. Modugno F, Ribechini E, Colombini MP (2006) Chemical study of triterpenoid resinous materials in archaeological findings by means of direct exposure electron ionisation mass spectrometry and gas chromatography/mass spectrometry. Rapid Commun Mass Spectrom 20: 1787–1800. doi: 10.1002/rcm.2507
  112. 112. Koller J, Baumer U, Mania D (2001) High-Tech in the Middle Palaeolithic: Neandertal-Manufactured Pitch Identified. European Journal of Archaeology 4: 385–397. doi: 10.1179/eja.2001.4.3.385
  113. 113. Grünberg JM (2002) Middle Palaeolithic birch bark pitch. Antiquity 76: 15–16.
  114. 114. Bar-Yosef Mayer DE, Vandermeersch B, Bar-Yosef O (2009) Shells and ochre in Middle Paleolithic Qafzeh Cave, Israel: indications for modern behavior. J Hum Evol 56: 307–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.10.005
  115. 115. Shea JJ (2003) The Middle Paleolithic of the East Mediterranean Levant. Journal of World Prehistory 17: 313–394. doi: 10.1023/b:jowo.0000020194.01496.fe
  116. 116. Villa P, Delagnes A, Wadley L (2005) A late Middle Stone Age artifact assemblage from Sibudu (KwaZulu-Natal): comparisons with the European Middle Paleolithic. J Archaeol Sci 32: 399–422. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2004.11.007
  117. 117. Klein RG, Steele TE (2013) Archaeological shellfish size and later human evolution in Africa Proc Natl Acad Sci. 110: 10910–10915. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1304750110
  118. 118. Zilhão J (2007) The emergence of ornaments and art: an archaeological perspective on the origins of “behavioral modernity”. J Archaeol Res 15: 1–54. doi: 10.1007/s10814-006-9008-1
  119. 119. Bar-Yosef O, Kuhn SL (1999) The big deal about blades: Laminar technologies and human evolution. Am Anthropol 101: 322–338. doi: 10.1525/aa.1999.101.2.322
  120. 120. Faivre JP (2012) A material anecdote but technical reality. Bladelet and small blade production during the recent Middle Paleolithic at Combe Grenal rock shelter. Lithic Technology 37: 5–25.
  121. 121. Slimak L (2008) The Neronian and the historical structure of cultural shifts from Middle to Upper Palaeolithic in Mediterranean France. J Archaeol Sci 35: 2204–2214. doi: 10.1016/j.jas.2008.02.005
  122. 122. Tostevin GB (2007) Social intimacy, artefact visibility and acculturation models of Neanderthal-modern human interaction. In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C, editors. Rethinking the human revolution. New behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp341–357.
  123. 123. Roussel M (2011) Normes et variations de la production lithique durant le Châtelperronien: la séquence de la Grande-Roche-de-la-Plématrie à Quinçay (Vienne). Université Paris Ouest, Nanterre, 566 p.
  124. 124. Lowe J, Barton N, Blockeley S, Bronk Ramsey C, Cullen VL, et al. (2012) Volcanic ash layers illuminate the resilience of Neanderthals and early modern humans to natural hazards. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109: 13471–13472.
  125. 125. Rougier H, Milota S, Rodrigo R, Gherase M, Sarcina L, et al. (2007) Peştera cu Oase 2 and the cranial morphology of early modern Europeans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104: 1165–1170. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610538104
  126. 126. Crevecoeur I, Bayle P, Rougier H, Maureille B, Higham T, et al. (2010) The Spy VI child: A newly discovered Neandertal infant. J Hum Evol 59: 641–656. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.07.022
  127. 127. Higham T, Bronk Ramsey C, Karavaniċ I, Smith FH, Trinakaus E (2006) Revised direct radiocarbon dating of the Vindija G1Upper Paleolithic Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci 103: 553–557. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510005103
  128. 128. Pinhasi R, Higham TFG, Golovanova LV, Doronichev VB (2011) Revised age of late Neanderthal occupation and the end of the Middle Paleolithic in the northern Caucasus. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108: 8611–8616. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018938108
  129. 129. White M, Pettitt P (2012) Ancient Digs and Modern Myths: The Age and Context of the Kent’s Cavern 4 Maxilla and the Earliest Homo sapiens Specimens in Europe. European Journal of Archaeology 15: 1–30. doi: 10.1179/1461957112y.0000000019
  130. 130. Rink WJ, Schwarcz HP, Lee HK, Rees-Jones J, Rabinovich R, et al. (2001) Electron spin resonance (ESR) and thermal ionization mass spectrometric (TIMS) 230Th/234U dating of teeth in Middle Paleolithic layers at Amud Cave, Israel. Geoarchaeology 16: 701–717. doi: 10.1002/gea.1017
  131. 131. Mellars P (2004) Neanderthals and the modern human colonization of Europe. Nature 432: 461–465. doi: 10.1038/nature03103
  132. 132. Condemi S, Mounier A, Giunti P, Lari M, Caramelli D, et al.. (2013) Possible Interbreeding in Late Italian Neanderthals? New Data from the Mezzena Jaw (Monti Lessini, Verona, Italy). PLoS One 8(3) e59781.
  133. 133. Smith F (2013) The fate of the Neanderthals. J Anthropol Res 69: 167–100.
  134. 134. Trinkaus E (2007) European early modern humans and the fate of the Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104: 7367–7372. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702214104
  135. 135. Zilhão J, Trinkaus E (2002) Portrait of the artist as a child. The Gravettian human skeleton from the Abrigo do Lagar Velho and its archaeological context. Trabalhos de Arqueologia 22. Lisboa, Instituto Portuguės de Arqueologia 610 p.
  136. 136. Mendez FL, Watkins JC, Hammer MF (2013) Neandertal Origin of Genetic Variation at the Cluster of OAS Immunity Genes. Mol Biol Evol 30: 798–801. doi: 10.1093/molbev/mst004
  137. 137. Ding Q, Hu Y, Xu S, Wang J, Jin L (2013) Neanderthal Introgression at Chromosome 3p21.31 was Under Positive Natural Selection in East Asians. Mol Biol Evol doi:10.1093/molbev/mst260.
  138. 138. Sankararaman S, Patterson N, Li H, Pääbo S, Reich D (2012) The Date of Interbreeding between Neandertals and Modern Humans. PLoS Genet 8(10) e1002947. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002947
  139. 139. Smith FH, Falsetti A, Donnelly S (1989) Modern human origins. Yearb Phys Anthropol 32: 35–68. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330320504
  140. 140. Smith FH (2011) Assimilation revisited: Africans, Neandertals and the origins of modern Eurasians. General Anthropology 18: 1–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-3466.2011.00009.x
  141. 141. Fu Q, Rudan P, Pääbo S, Krause J (2012) Complete Mitochondrial Genomes Reveal Neolithic Expansion into Europe. PLoS One 7(3) e32473.
  142. 142. Bollongino R, Nehlich O, Richards M, Orschiedt J, Thomas MG, et al. (2013) 2000 Years of Parallel Societies in Stone Age Central Europe. Science 342: 479–481. doi: 10.1126/science.1245049
  143. 143. Brandt G, Haak W, Adler CJ, Roth C, Szécsényi-Nagy A, et al. (2013) Ancient DNA Reveals Key Stages in the Formation of Central European Mitochondrial Genetic Diversity. Science 342: 257–261. doi: 10.1126/science.1241844
  144. 144. Sankararaman S, Mallick S, Dannemann M, Prüfer K, Kelso J, et al. (2014) The genomic landscape of Neanderthal ancestry in present-day humans. Nature 507: 354–357. doi: 10.1038/nature12961
  145. 145. Gibbons A (2014) Oldest Homo sapiens genome pinpoints Neandertal input. Science 343: 1417. doi: 10.1126/science.343.6178.1417
  146. 146. Briggs AW, Good JM, Green RE, Krause J, KMaricic T, et al. (2009) Targeted Retrieval and Analysis of Five Neandertal mtDNA Genomes. Science 325: 318–321. doi: 10.1126/science.1174462
  147. 147. Straus LG (2005) A mosaic of change: the Middle-Upper Paleolithic transition as viewed from New Mexico and Iberia. Quat Int 137: 47–67. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2004.11.019
  148. 148. Banks WE, d’Errico F, Townsend Peterson A, Kageyama M, Sima A, et al. (2008) Neanderthal extinction by competitive exclusion. PLoS One 3(12): e3972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003972
  149. 149. Gamble C (2011) Neuroscience: Neanderthals in mind. Nature 479: 294–295. doi: 10.1038/479294a
  150. 150. Pearce E, Stringer C, Dunbar RIM (2013) New insights into differences in brain organization between Neanderthals and anatomically modern humans. Proc R Soc B 280: 20130168. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0168
  151. 151. Stringer C (2012) Evolution: What makes a modern human. Nature 85: 33–35.

*** 

Source:

http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0096424




Fair Use Notice

This site contains copyrighted material the use of which has not always been specifically authorized by the copyright owner. We are making such material available in our efforts to advance understanding of environmental, political, human rights, economic, democracy, scientific, and social justice issues, etc. We believe this constitutes a 'fair use' of any such copyrighted material as provided for in section 107 of the US Copyright Law. In accordance with Title 17 U.S.C. Section 107, the material on this site is distributed without profit to those who have expressed a prior interest in receiving the included information for research and educational purposes. For more information go to: http://www.law.cornell.edu/uscode/17/107.shtml. If you wish to use copyrighted material from this site for purposes of your own that go beyond 'fair use', you must obtain permission from the copyright owner.

 

 

 

Opinions expressed in various sections are the sole responsibility of their authors and they may not represent Al-Jazeerah & ccun.org.

editor@aljazeerah.info & editor@ccun.org